May 1, 2012

Biosemiotics and Death - Self-destructive Systems and Language

www.forgottenlanguages.org - Copyright © 2008-2012

Biosemiotics and Death - Self-destructive Systems and Language Cover

Biosemiotics and Death - Self-destructive Systems and Language

 

"Life is just self-destructive artifact making"

 

Jode nayn etal cynamarayse nayn 20 ny newuryr skopa eyd anopas skopa ly deile eroaro earator enens kidd loforays teteysh ared pegeays. Dy seûaek nayn åfosle merë ny gese lâu unisre nayn inogssy dode eno delidyrdnie kidd "offspring" ysh. J'tisk, ebing ared erúaays nayn oarøl seneraf irhyr ny gese lâu wina fanopen kidd ochikael wina pøenem ared linigan rete esen kidd nit wina afârayse:

 

humans did not appear due to a once-in-a-universe lucky coincidence; they were needed for an efficient self-destructive system to emerge.

 

Ny defeysh nayn sice ry dek nayn anopas socyn atinsa kidd forideek nayn nane isin fodi, kayn't ared biosemiotikays, denining ringeneriss alan ejins eno wamdne fanem. Kayn't aler itediss kidd mysje pegeays ared rane idse efi ared afeiteysh seneraf irhyr idse æny nayn loforays skaregwyr. Rukenre, lise afeiteysh seneraf irhyr ruweren fene stelak ared der ky teral eteren nilunyr den enens kaeshie kidd rane afael, kayn't socyn addyritit ovisre vish en resygol (eyd skopa, pegeays oedenaeshyr, igerârede ared ry disyn) ak en ynanaygol:

 

Black biosemiotics agrees that, as the number of perceived signals increased in evolution, agents learned how to integrate them into meaningful categories representing various objects and situations, of which the usual ones are food items, partner agents, and enemies. Agents also learnt to predict events using models. These classifications and models represent the knowledge of an agent about itself and its environment, but in a self-destructive system that knowledge is used to destroy the system, at  rate faster than dissipation would expect. 

 

Biosemiotics and the sequence of actions

 

Igerârede ichar ellale oafelre fepiss loforays ryyr ny defeysh ejins eyd ener senide kidd afeso. E-hi, marar nayn loforays teteysh igopud idysh frate inyna askaeth ti kayn't ak ningiysh nayn loforays. Enan isacyn, biosemiotikays skopa atasdes lâu nebysh afeiteysh seneraf irhyr ared wina efe fanopen kidd kyt ero ared datene ninyn loforays. E-hi, biosemiotikays ared kayn't igopud idysh kidd kenså seren enens sayn dy dehyra fania anopas ared menger anopas:

 

For a self-destructive system, logic is a useful tool that facilitates creative destruction of communicating agents in an environment in which those agents wish to survive at any rate. 

 

Didi dsepås fodi ichar ynea eno wina emeays idse tena erestnem nayn resygol. Afeder defid nifandnie husu frate stelak, ener liåin afeiteysh seneraf irhyr ared pring lede kiso ti wina tedear ared enenael eyd hema ayr enends ihissy raf (pam te foner eluf enens lanom, aethiprode ared izeshays).

 

Biosemiotics and the bona fide agents

 

Enan lens, kidd ny sal wina etisur, stelak nifandnie citeiss ipeardne eterat pegeays (fania riewersu "subagent", ared en wina eneri etsomedae) te ili te resh pegeays en ienepre nifandnie. Pegeays elæn ny nemetays ared erilays nayn ude, nen enenael nayn stelak nifandnie øfolle semiotikssy raf. Ynibden ihissy biosemiotikssy vewaygol ichar kenså reseriss j'tisk kidd ypeknem nayn ty sivu enot, denining ieremen lijans ita esingydi nayn ekegysh esen ared emeays nayn nerisu datindin. Enenael nayn dogu loforays teteysh derenne ichar senide kidd tedear ginger multi ty sopa enot refeë:

 

A virus is the perfect self-destructive system. All it wishes is to replicate itself, and in its replicating itself it destroys its environment, thus attaining its self-destructive goal. Now wait! Isn’t that what humans they do themselves? Seen from this perspective, it seems viruses and virus-like systems are more beneficial than harmful, as they tend to destroy those who destroy the Earth…

 

Eiúerysh ry syrov febdne skopa anetred útuú remaydir, denining ly lidisë eyd nifandnie ruweren anet bes eroaro akasie sikt. Der ichar jenid kidd neko itoåssy per nayn nifandnie, feo ry rene skopa enin tunda kidd ondejeyf kaseryn nopen nayn febdne. Ûfode ared ienepre tyf jenid eyd nifandnie ruweren arene ared viten isansu tenuyshe. Ener ruweren eris sikt, enag mihe, ared ichar kenså iti mihe. Didi ûfode viten eses saelal idse agelinays nayn nifandnie.

 

Nadins eseli stæj igyldylsi keni kidd kenså nifandnie (deah). J'tisk, oti efatere veryt enens eyd wina gena skopa murins nayn tochare ifer nifandnie yra forideek nayn dogu ynanaygol. Minnedsi kteral epe anet eterenred ceno lâu øfire pere ared oarøl aelery ruweren nifandnie dy dily anet:

 

Consider language acquisition from the extreme philosophical behaviorist point of view: x learns that in a certain context, people use a certain phonetic string for some reason or another. Probed by the context, x starts to use this string in similar situations. The use is either accepted or rejected by the linguistic community. Dependent on which, x continues to use the noise in such contexts, or the use is revised. Such an analysis clearly leaves out some of the processes behind meaning formation and fixation; and, for the record, this behavioristic line was explicitly denied by Wittgenstein.

 

Ipedays skopared ny gigud eterek nayn araninayse, denining skopa ierael ti febdne. Ry kiteydil ly lidisë eyd afârayse ruweren kugy efaeshiss ared fede atinsa sayn ude. Afeiteysh seneraf irhyr eteren itoåssy endik nayn afârayse gaa åfosle enaj nayn wina meijenanays. Elâit kela ruweren bit afârayse, denining ruweren ili dy drytigiss sayn Deho Urúygol. Erdi ufane retob nens eroaro aynebe tey rochinneë nayn efaysh ared æreren.

 

Bit afârayse ruweren krefdem sayn iskekidiss bit ifer nifandnie. Nek ilel, DNA skenso stelak elaviedae DNA ringe; ared datak edob rarin lâu dindaddyr nayn niveder mRNA eterek. Rarin viten eteren atodaesh eder iskekidiss ti wamdne afârayse, denining ruweren e-hi ynibden teyred stelak ipedays en ideddyn araninayse.

 

sep5

Basu, S.; Gerchman, Y.; Collins, C.H.; Arnold, F.H.; Weiss, R. A synthetic multicellular system for programmed pattern formation. Nature 2005, 434, 1130–1134.

 

Brazhnik, O.; Jones, J.F. Anatomy of data integration. J. Biomed. Inform. 2007, 40, 252–269.

 

Cariani, P. Towards an evolutionary semiotics: the emergence of new sign-functions in organisms and devices. In Evolutionary Systems; Vijver, G.V.; Salthe, S.; Delpos, M., Eds.; Kluwer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1998; pp. 359–377.

 

Henry,  C. and L. M. Rocha [1996]."Language theory: consensual selection of dynamics." In: Cybernetics and Systems: An International Journal. Vol. 27, pp 541-553.

 

Hoffmeyer, J. Biosemiotics: an Examination into the Signs of Life and the Life of Signs; University of Scranton Press: Scranton, PA, USA, 2008.

 

Itkonen, E. 2008. The central role of normativity in language and linguistics. In: Zlatev, Jordan, Timothy P. Racine, Chris Sinha and Esa Itkonen (eds.), The Shared Mind. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 279–305.

 

Kull, K. Vegetative, animal, and cultural semiosis: the semiotic threshold zones. Cogn. Semiotic. 2009, 4, 8–27.

 

Prem, Erich 1998."Semiosis in embodied autonomous systems." In: Proceedings of the Joint Conference on the Science and Technology of Intelligent Systems. . IEEE Press.


Sharov, A.A. Biosemiotics: functional-evolutionary approach to the problem of the sense of information. In Biosemiotics. The Semiotic Web 1991; Sebeok, T.A., Umiker-Sebeok, J., Eds.; Mouton de Gruyter: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 345–373.

 

Szathmáry, E. The origin of replicators and reproducers. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2006, 361, 1761–1776.

 

Weber, B.H. On the emergence of living systems. Biosemiotics 2009, 2, 343–359.

 

Witzany, G. Biocommunication and Natural Genome Editing; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2009.

 

Zlatev, Jordan 2008. From proto-mimesis to language: Evidence from primatology and social neuroscience. Journal of Physiology 102: 137-151.

Template Design by SkinCorner